File Download
There are no files associated with this item.
Links for fulltext
(May Require Subscription)
- Publisher Website: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.005
- Scopus: eid_2-s2.0-78149483901
- PMID: 21074112
- WOS: WOS:000284519800021
- Find via
Supplementary
- Citations:
- Appears in Collections:
Article: Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill
Title | Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill |
---|---|
Authors | |
Keywords | Cognitive demand Errors Golf Movement preparation Probe reaction time |
Issue Date | 2010 |
Publisher | Academic Press. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.elsevier.com/locate/concog |
Citation | Consciousness And Cognition, 2010, v. 19 n. 4, p. 1058-1061 How to Cite? |
Abstract | Maxwell et al. [Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., Kerr, E., & Weedon, E. (2001). The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1049-1068. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1049-1068] suggested that, following unsuccessful movements, the learner forms hypotheses about the probable causes of the error and the required movement adjustments necessary for its elimination. Hypothesis testing is an explicit process that places demands on cognitive resources. Demands on cognitive resources can be identified by measuring probe reaction times (PRT) and movement times. Lengthened PRT and movement times reflects increased cognitive demands. Thus, PRT and movement times should be longer following errors, relative to successful, movements. This hypothesis was tested using a motor skill (golf putting). Furthermore, the association between error processing and the preparation and execution phases of movement was examined. The data confirmed that cognitive demand is greater for trials following an error, relative to trials without an error. This effect was apparent throughout learning and in both the preparatory and execution phases of the movement. Cognitive effort also appeared to be higher during movement preparation, relative to movement execution. © 2008. |
Persistent Identifier | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/176066 |
ISSN | 2023 Impact Factor: 2.1 2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.827 |
ISI Accession Number ID | |
References |
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Lam, WK | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Masters, RSW | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Maxwell, JP | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2012-11-26T09:04:50Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2012-11-26T09:04:50Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 2010 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Consciousness And Cognition, 2010, v. 19 n. 4, p. 1058-1061 | en_US |
dc.identifier.issn | 1053-8100 | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/176066 | - |
dc.description.abstract | Maxwell et al. [Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., Kerr, E., & Weedon, E. (2001). The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1049-1068. The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1049-1068] suggested that, following unsuccessful movements, the learner forms hypotheses about the probable causes of the error and the required movement adjustments necessary for its elimination. Hypothesis testing is an explicit process that places demands on cognitive resources. Demands on cognitive resources can be identified by measuring probe reaction times (PRT) and movement times. Lengthened PRT and movement times reflects increased cognitive demands. Thus, PRT and movement times should be longer following errors, relative to successful, movements. This hypothesis was tested using a motor skill (golf putting). Furthermore, the association between error processing and the preparation and execution phases of movement was examined. The data confirmed that cognitive demand is greater for trials following an error, relative to trials without an error. This effect was apparent throughout learning and in both the preparatory and execution phases of the movement. Cognitive effort also appeared to be higher during movement preparation, relative to movement execution. © 2008. | en_US |
dc.language | eng | en_US |
dc.publisher | Academic Press. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.elsevier.com/locate/concog | en_US |
dc.relation.ispartof | Consciousness and Cognition | en_US |
dc.subject | Cognitive demand | - |
dc.subject | Errors | - |
dc.subject | Golf | - |
dc.subject | Movement preparation | - |
dc.subject | Probe reaction time | - |
dc.subject.mesh | Adolescent | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Attention | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Cognition | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Distance Perception | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Female | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Humans | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Male | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Motor Skills | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Orientation | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Psychomotor Performance | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Reaction Time | en_US |
dc.subject.mesh | Young Adult | en_US |
dc.title | Cognitive demands of error processing associated with preparation and execution of a motor skill | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.identifier.email | Masters, RSW: mastersr@hku.hk | en_US |
dc.identifier.authority | Masters, RSW=rp00935 | en_US |
dc.description.nature | link_to_subscribed_fulltext | en_US |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.005 | en_US |
dc.identifier.pmid | 21074112 | - |
dc.identifier.scopus | eid_2-s2.0-78149483901 | en_US |
dc.identifier.hkuros | 183319 | - |
dc.relation.references | http://www.scopus.com/mlt/select.url?eid=2-s2.0-78149483901&selection=ref&src=s&origin=recordpage | en_US |
dc.identifier.volume | 19 | en_US |
dc.identifier.issue | 4 | en_US |
dc.identifier.spage | 1058 | en_US |
dc.identifier.epage | 1061 | en_US |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:000284519800021 | - |
dc.publisher.place | United States | en_US |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Lam, WK=35237823600 | en_US |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Masters, RSW=7102880488 | en_US |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Maxwell, JP=7201610565 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citeulike | 8339948 | - |
dc.identifier.issnl | 1053-8100 | - |