File Download
  Links for fulltext
     (May Require Subscription)
Supplementary

Article: Both parallelism and orthogonality are used to perceive 3D slant of rectangles from 2D images

TitleBoth parallelism and orthogonality are used to perceive 3D slant of rectangles from 2D images
Authors
KeywordsDepth perception
Linear perspective
Picture perception
Skew symmetry
Slant perception
Vision
Issue Date2007
PublisherAssociation for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology. The Journal's web site is located at http://wwwjournalofvisionorg/
Citation
Journal Of Vision, 2007, v. 7 n. 6, article no. 7 How to Cite?
AbstractA 2D perspective image of a slanted rectangular object is sufficient for a strong 3D percept. Two computational assumptions that could be used to interpret 3D from images of rectangles are as follows: (1) converging lines in an image are parallel in the world, and (2) skewed angles in an image are orthogonal in the world. For an accurate perspective image of a slanted rectangle, either constraint implies the same 3D interpretation. However, if an image is rescaled, the 3D interpretations based on parallelism and orthogonality generally conflict. We tested the roles of parallelism and orthogonality by measuring perceived depth within scaled perspective images. Stimuli were monocular images of squares, slanted about a horizontal axis, with an elliptical hole. Subjects judged the length-to-width ratio of the holes, which provided a measure of perceived depth along the object. The rotational alignment of squares within their surface plane was varied from 0° (trapezoidal projected contours) to 20° (skewed projected contours). In consistent-cue conditions, images were accurate projections of either a 10° or 20°-wide square, with slants of 75° and 62°, respectively. In cue-conflict conditions, images were generated either by magnifying a 10° image to have a projected size of 20° or by minifying a 20° image to have a projected size of 10°. For the aligned squares, which do not produce a conflicting skew cue, we found that subjects, judgments depended primarily on projected size and not on the size used to generate the prescaled images. This is consistent with reliance on the convergence cue, corresponding to a parallelism assumption. As squares were rotated away from alignment, producing skewed projected contours, judgments were increasingly determined by the original image size. This is consistent with use of the skew cue, corresponding to an orthogonality assumption. Our results demonstrate that both parallelism and orthogonality constraints are used to perceive depth from linear perspective. © ARVO.
Persistent Identifierhttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/169023
ISSN
2023 Impact Factor: 2.0
2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.849
ISI Accession Number ID
References

 

DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorSaunders, JAen_US
dc.contributor.authorBackus, BTen_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-10-08T03:40:51Z-
dc.date.available2012-10-08T03:40:51Z-
dc.date.issued2007en_US
dc.identifier.citationJournal Of Vision, 2007, v. 7 n. 6, article no. 7en_US
dc.identifier.issn1534-7362en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/169023-
dc.description.abstractA 2D perspective image of a slanted rectangular object is sufficient for a strong 3D percept. Two computational assumptions that could be used to interpret 3D from images of rectangles are as follows: (1) converging lines in an image are parallel in the world, and (2) skewed angles in an image are orthogonal in the world. For an accurate perspective image of a slanted rectangle, either constraint implies the same 3D interpretation. However, if an image is rescaled, the 3D interpretations based on parallelism and orthogonality generally conflict. We tested the roles of parallelism and orthogonality by measuring perceived depth within scaled perspective images. Stimuli were monocular images of squares, slanted about a horizontal axis, with an elliptical hole. Subjects judged the length-to-width ratio of the holes, which provided a measure of perceived depth along the object. The rotational alignment of squares within their surface plane was varied from 0° (trapezoidal projected contours) to 20° (skewed projected contours). In consistent-cue conditions, images were accurate projections of either a 10° or 20°-wide square, with slants of 75° and 62°, respectively. In cue-conflict conditions, images were generated either by magnifying a 10° image to have a projected size of 20° or by minifying a 20° image to have a projected size of 10°. For the aligned squares, which do not produce a conflicting skew cue, we found that subjects, judgments depended primarily on projected size and not on the size used to generate the prescaled images. This is consistent with reliance on the convergence cue, corresponding to a parallelism assumption. As squares were rotated away from alignment, producing skewed projected contours, judgments were increasingly determined by the original image size. This is consistent with use of the skew cue, corresponding to an orthogonality assumption. Our results demonstrate that both parallelism and orthogonality constraints are used to perceive depth from linear perspective. © ARVO.en_US
dc.languageengen_US
dc.publisherAssociation for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology. The Journal's web site is located at http://wwwjournalofvisionorg/en_US
dc.relation.ispartofJournal of Visionen_US
dc.subjectDepth perception-
dc.subjectLinear perspective-
dc.subjectPicture perception-
dc.subjectSkew symmetry-
dc.subjectSlant perception-
dc.subjectVision-
dc.subject.meshAdulten_US
dc.subject.meshCuesen_US
dc.subject.meshDepth Perceptionen_US
dc.subject.meshHumansen_US
dc.subject.meshMaleen_US
dc.subject.meshModels, Psychologicalen_US
dc.subject.meshPhotic Stimulation - Methodsen_US
dc.subject.meshPsychophysicsen_US
dc.titleBoth parallelism and orthogonality are used to perceive 3D slant of rectangles from 2D imagesen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.identifier.emailSaunders, JA:jsaun@hkucc.hku.hken_US
dc.identifier.authoritySaunders, JA=rp00638en_US
dc.description.naturelink_to_OA_fulltexten_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1167/7.6.7en_US
dc.identifier.pmid17685790-
dc.identifier.scopuseid_2-s2.0-34247102856en_US
dc.relation.referenceshttp://www.scopus.com/mlt/select.url?eid=2-s2.0-34247102856&selection=ref&src=s&origin=recordpageen_US
dc.identifier.volume7en_US
dc.identifier.issue6en_US
dc.identifier.spagearticle no. 7-
dc.identifier.epagearticle no. 7-
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000251614700007-
dc.publisher.placeUnited Statesen_US
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridSaunders, JA=7402341514en_US
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridBackus, BT=7003366612en_US
dc.identifier.issnl1534-7362-

Export via OAI-PMH Interface in XML Formats


OR


Export to Other Non-XML Formats