File Download
  Links for fulltext
     (May Require Subscription)
Supplementary

Article: No transfer of calibration between action and perception in learning a golf putting task

TitleNo transfer of calibration between action and perception in learning a golf putting task
Authors
KeywordsCalibration
Far aiming
Perceptual learning
Transfer
Two-visual-system model
Issue Date2011
PublisherSpringer New York
Citation
Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 2011, v. 73, n. 7, p. 2298-2308 How to Cite?
Persistent Identifierhttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/147140
ISSN
2023 Impact Factor: 1.7
2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.833
ISI Accession Number ID
References

Blakemore, S. J., & Frith, C. (2005). The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuropsychologia, 43, 260–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.012

Cuijpers, R. H., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2000). Investigation of visual space using an exocentric pointing task. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1556–1571. doi: 10.3758/BF03212156

Cuijpers, R. H., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2003). The metrics of visual and haptic space based on parallelity judgements. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 47, 278–291. doi: 10.1016/S0022-2496(03)00004-X

Dicks, M., Button, C., & Davids, K. (2010). Examination of gaze behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 706–720. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.3.706

Ernst, M. O., & Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature, 415, 429–433. doi: 10.1038/415429a

Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20–25. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00209

Gibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 473–482. doi: 10.1037/h0055007

Gonzalez, C. L. R., Ganel, T., Whitwell, R. L., Morrissey, B., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: Sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychologia, 46, 624–631. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.006

Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103

Jacobs, D. M., & Michaels, C. F. (2007). Direct learning. Ecological Psychology, 19, 321–349. doi: 10.1080/10407410701432337

Johnston, A., Benton, C. P., & Nishida, S. (2003). Golfers may have to overcome a persistent visuospatial illusion. Perception, 32, 1151–1154. doi: 10.1068/p5056

Koedijker, J. M., Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., Oudejans, R. D. D., Beek, P. J., & Masters, R. S. W. (2011). Attention and time constraints in perceptual–motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 245–256. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.08.002

Koenderink, J. J., van Doorn, A. J., & Lappin, J. S. (2000). Direct measurement of curvature of visual space. Perception, 29, 69–79. doi: 10.1068/p2921

Koenderink, J. J., van Doorn, A. J., & Lappin, J. S. (2003). Exocentric pointing to opposite targets. Acta Psychologica, 112, 71–87. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00101-4

Martin, T. A., Keating, J. G., Goodkin, H. P., Bastian, A. J., & Thach, W. T. (1996). Throwing while looking through prisms: I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain, 119, 1183–1198. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1183

Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774–785. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. doi: 10.1080/713752551

Prinz, W. (2006). What re-enactment earns us. Cortex, 42, 515–517. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70389-7

Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual–motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 21, 480–497. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.480

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

Roberts, R., & Turnbull, O. H. (2010). Putts that get missed on the right: Investigating lateralized attentional biases and the nature of putting errors in golf. Journal of Sport Sciences, 28, 369–374. doi: 10.1080/02640410903536467

Van der Kamp, J., Oudejans, R. D. D., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2003). The development and learning of the visual control of movement: An ecological perspective. Infant Behavior & Development, 26, 495–515. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002

Wagman, J. B., McBride, D. M., & Trefzger, A. J. (2008). Perceptual experience and posttest improvements in perceptual accuracy and consistency. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1060–1067. doi: 10.3758/PP.70.6.1060

Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.558

Withagen, R., & Caljouw, S. R. (2011). Aging affects attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1216–1226. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0092-z

Withagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2002). The calibration of walking transfers to crawling: Are action systems calibrated? Ecological Psychology, 14, 223–234. doi: 10.1207/S15326969ECO1404_2

Withagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2004). Transfer of calibration in length perception by dynamic touch. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1282–1292. doi: 10.3758/BF03194998

Withagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2005). The role of feedback information for calibration and attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1379–1390. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1379

 

DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorVan Lier, Wen_US
dc.contributor.authorVan der Kamp, Jen_US
dc.contributor.authorvan der Zanden, Aen_US
dc.contributor.authorSavelsbergh, GJen_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-05-28T08:20:52Z-
dc.date.available2012-05-28T08:20:52Z-
dc.date.issued2011en_US
dc.identifier.citationAttention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 2011, v. 73, n. 7, p. 2298-2308en_US
dc.identifier.issn1943-3921en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/147140-
dc.languageengen_US
dc.publisherSpringer New Yorken_US
dc.relation.ispartofAttention, Perception, & Psychophysicsen_US
dc.rightsThe Author(s)en_US
dc.rightsThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.en_US
dc.subjectCalibration-
dc.subjectFar aiming-
dc.subjectPerceptual learning-
dc.subjectTransfer-
dc.subjectTwo-visual-system model-
dc.titleNo transfer of calibration between action and perception in learning a golf putting tasken_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.identifier.openurlhttp://www.springerlink.com/link-out/?id=2104&code=119G5871J0348422&MUD=MPen_US
dc.description.naturepublished_or_final_versionen_US
dc.identifier.doi10.3758/s13414-011-0184-9en_US
dc.identifier.pmid21814859-
dc.identifier.scopuseid_2-s2.0-80155187448en_US
dc.relation.referencesBlakemore, S. J., & Frith, C. (2005). The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuropsychologia, 43, 260–267.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.012en_US
dc.relation.referencesCabe, P. A., & Wagman, J. B. (2010). Characterizing perceptual learning using regression statistics: Development of a perceptual calibration index. The American Journal of Psychology, 123, 253–267.en_US
dc.relation.referencesCuijpers, R. H., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2000). Investigation of visual space using an exocentric pointing task. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1556–1571.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.3758/BF03212156en_US
dc.relation.referencesCuijpers, R. H., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (2003). The metrics of visual and haptic space based on parallelity judgements. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 47, 278–291.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/S0022-2496(03)00004-Xen_US
dc.relation.referencesDicks, M., Button, C., & Davids, K. (2010). Examination of gaze behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 706–720.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.3758/APP.72.3.706en_US
dc.relation.referencesErnst, M. O., & Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature, 415, 429–433.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1038/415429aen_US
dc.relation.referencesFranz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20–25.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00209en_US
dc.relation.referencesGibson, E. J., & Bergman, R. (1954). The effect of training on absolute estimation of distance over the ground. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 473–482.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1037/h0055007en_US
dc.relation.referencesGonzalez, C. L. R., Ganel, T., Whitwell, R. L., Morrissey, B., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: Sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychologia, 46, 624–631.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.006en_US
dc.relation.referencesHommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103en_US
dc.relation.referencesJacobs, D. M., & Michaels, C. F. (2007). Direct learning. Ecological Psychology, 19, 321–349.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1080/10407410701432337en_US
dc.relation.referencesJohnston, A., Benton, C. P., & Nishida, S. (2003). Golfers may have to overcome a persistent visuospatial illusion. Perception, 32, 1151–1154.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1068/p5056en_US
dc.relation.referencesKoedijker, J. M., Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., Oudejans, R. D. D., Beek, P. J., & Masters, R. S. W. (2011). Attention and time constraints in perceptual–motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 245–256.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.08.002en_US
dc.relation.referencesKoenderink, J. J., van Doorn, A. J., & Lappin, J. S. (2000). Direct measurement of curvature of visual space. Perception, 29, 69–79.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1068/p2921en_US
dc.relation.referencesKoenderink, J. J., van Doorn, A. J., & Lappin, J. S. (2003). Exocentric pointing to opposite targets. Acta Psychologica, 112, 71–87.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00101-4en_US
dc.relation.referencesMartin, T. A., Keating, J. G., Goodkin, H. P., Bastian, A. J., & Thach, W. T. (1996). Throwing while looking through prisms: I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain, 119, 1183–1198.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1183en_US
dc.relation.referencesMilner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46, 774–785.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005en_US
dc.relation.referencesPrinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1080/713752551en_US
dc.relation.referencesPrinz, W. (2006). What re-enactment earns us. Cortex, 42, 515–517.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70389-7en_US
dc.relation.referencesRieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual–motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 21, 480–497.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.480en_US
dc.relation.referencesRizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 27, 169–192.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230en_US
dc.relation.referencesRoberts, R., & Turnbull, O. H. (2010). Putts that get missed on the right: Investigating lateralized attentional biases and the nature of putting errors in golf. Journal of Sport Sciences, 28, 369–374.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1080/02640410903536467en_US
dc.relation.referencesVan der Kamp, J., Oudejans, R. D. D., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2003). The development and learning of the visual control of movement: An ecological perspective. Infant Behavior & Development, 26, 495–515.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002en_US
dc.relation.referencesWagman, J. B., McBride, D. M., & Trefzger, A. J. (2008). Perceptual experience and posttest improvements in perceptual accuracy and consistency. Perception & Psychophysics, 70, 1060–1067.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.3758/PP.70.6.1060en_US
dc.relation.referencesWillingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.558en_US
dc.relation.referencesWithagen, R., & Caljouw, S. R. (2011). Aging affects attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1216–1226.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0092-zen_US
dc.relation.referencesWithagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2002). The calibration of walking transfers to crawling: Are action systems calibrated? Ecological Psychology, 14, 223–234.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1207/S15326969ECO1404_2en_US
dc.relation.referencesWithagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2004). Transfer of calibration in length perception by dynamic touch. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1282–1292.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.3758/BF03194998en_US
dc.relation.referencesWithagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2005). The role of feedback information for calibration and attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1379–1390.en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1379en_US
dc.relation.referencesAbernethy, B., Thomas, K. T., & Thomas, J. T. (1993). Strategies for improving understanding of motor expertise (or mistakes we have made and things we have learned!). In J. Starkes & F. Allard (Eds.), Cognitive issues in motor expertise (pp 317–358). New York: North-Holland.en_US
dc.relation.referencesCavina-Pratesi, C., Kuhn, G., Ietswaart, M., & Milner, A. D. (2011). The magic grasp: Motor expertise in deception. PlosONE, 6, 1–5.en_US
dc.relation.referencesDicks, M., Davids, K., & Button, C. (2009). Representative design for the study of perception and action in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40, 506–524.en_US
dc.relation.referencesKelso, A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge: MIT Press.en_US
dc.relation.referencesKoenderink, J. J., & van Doorn, A. J. (1998). Exocentric pointing. In L. R. Harris & M. Jenkin (Eds.), Vision and action (pp. 295–313). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.en_US
dc.relation.referencesMilner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.en_US
dc.relation.referencesRedding, G. M., & Wallace, B. (1997). Adaptive spatial alignment. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.en_US
dc.relation.referencesRowe, R. M., & McKenna, F. P. (2001). Skilled anticipation in real-world tasks: Measurements of attentional demands in the domain of tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 60–67.en_US
dc.relation.referencesSavelsbergh, G.J.P., van Gastel, P.J., & van Kampen, P.M. (2010) Anticipation of penalty kicking direction can be improved by directing attention through perceptual learning. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 41, 24–41.en_US
dc.relation.referencesSmeeton, N.J., Williams, A.M., Hodges, N.J., & Ward, P. (2005). The relative effectiveness of various instructional approaches in developing anticipation skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 98–110.en_US
dc.relation.referencesVan der Kamp, J., Rivas, F., van Doorn, H., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2008). Ventral and dorsal system contributions to visual anticipation in fast ball sports. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 39, 100–130.en_US
dc.relation.referencesVan Lier, W., Van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2011). Perception and action in golf putting: Skill differences reflect calibration. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30(33), 349–369.en_US
dc.relation.referencesWarren, W. H. (1988). The perception–action coupling. In H. Bloch & B. I. Bertenthal (Eds.), Sensory–motor organizations and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 23–37). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.en_US
dc.relation.referencesWilliams, A.M., Ward, P., & Chapman, C. (2003) Training perceptual skill in field hockey: Is there transfer from the laboratory to the field? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74, 98–103.en_US
dc.identifier.volume73en_US
dc.identifier.issue7en_US
dc.identifier.spage2298en_US
dc.identifier.epage2308en_US
dc.identifier.eissn1943-393Xen_US
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000296358100024-
dc.description.otherSpringer Open Choice, 28 May 2012en_US
dc.identifier.issnl1943-3921-

Export via OAI-PMH Interface in XML Formats


OR


Export to Other Non-XML Formats