File Download
  Links for fulltext
     (May Require Subscription)
Supplementary

Article: A Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception

TitleA Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception
Authors
KeywordsDorsal system
Far-aiming
Judd-illusion
Ventral system
Issue Date2009
PublisherSpringer Berlin / Heidelberg
Citation
Experimental Brain Research, 2009, v. 197, n. 2, p. 199-204 How to Cite?
AbstractThe present study addresses the role of vision for perception in determining the location of a target in far-aiming. Participants (N = 12) slid a disk toward a distant target embedded in illusory Judd figures. Additionally, in a perception task, participants indicated when a moving pointer reached the midpoint of the Judd figures. The number of hits, the number of misses to the left and to the right of the target, the sliding error (in mm) and perceptual judgment error (in mm) served as dependent variables. Results showed an illusory bias in sliding, the magnitude of which was comparable to the bias in the perception of target location. The determination of target location in far-aiming is thus based on relative metrics. We argue that vision for perception sets the boundary constraints for action and that within these constraints vision for action autonomously controls movement execution, but alternative accounts are discussed as well.
Persistent Identifierhttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/144952
ISSN
2023 Impact Factor: 1.7
2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.613
PubMed Central ID
ISI Accession Number ID
References

Agliotti S, DeSouza JF, Goodale MA (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Cur Biol 5:679–685 doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3

Bruno N, Bernardis P, Gentilucci M (2008) Visually guided pointing, the Müller-Lyer illusion, and the functional interpretation of the dorsal-ventral split: conclusions from 33 independent studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:423–437 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.006

Crajé C, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B (2008) The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task. Exp Brain Res 185:53–62 doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1130-x

Ellis RR, Flanagan JR, Lederman SJ (1999) The influence of visual illusions on grasp position. Exp Brain Res 125:109–114 doi: 10.1007/s002210050665

Fleming J, Behrmann M (1998) Visuospatial neglect in normal subjects: altered spatial representations induced by a perceptual illusion. Neuropsychologia 36:469–475 doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00131-0

Ganel T, Tanzer M, Goodale MA (2008) A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: opposite effects of real and illusory size. Psychol Sci 19:221–225 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02071.x

Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E et al (1996) Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 34:369–376 doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00128-X

Glover S, Dixon P (2004) A step and a hop on the Müller-Lyer illusion: illusion effects on lower-limb movements. Exp Brain Res 154:504–512 doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1687-y

Gonzalez CLR, Ganel T, Whitwell RL et al (2008) Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychologia 46:624–631 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.006

Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25 doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

Longo MR, Lourenco SF (2006) On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia 44:977–981 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003

McCourt ME, Garlinghouse M (2000) Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex 36:715–731 doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70548-3

Milner AD, Goodale MA (2008) Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46:774–785 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005

Post RB, Welch RB (1996) Is there dissociation of perceptual and motor responses to figural illusions? Percept 25:569–581 doi: 10.1068/p250569

Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cujpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7

van der Kamp J, Masters RSW (2008) The human Müller-Lyer illusion in goalkeeping. Percept 37:951–954 doi: 10.1068/p6010

van der Kamp J, Oudejans RDD, Savelsbergh GJP (2003) The development and learning of the visual control of movement: an ecological perspective. Inf Behav Dev 26:495–515 doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002

van Doorn H, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2007) Grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion: the contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 45:1939–1947 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008

Vickers JN (1992) Gaze control in putting. Percept 21:117–132 doi: 10.1068/p210117

Vishton PM, Pea JG, Cutting JE, Nunez LN (1999) Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25:1659–1672 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1659

Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spat Vis 16:243–254 doi: 10.1163/156856803322467518

 

DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorvan der Kamp, Jen_US
dc.contributor.authorvan Doorn, Hen_US
dc.contributor.authorMasters, RSWen_US
dc.date.accessioned2012-02-21T05:44:56Z-
dc.date.available2012-02-21T05:44:56Z-
dc.date.issued2009en_US
dc.identifier.citationExperimental Brain Research, 2009, v. 197, n. 2, p. 199-204en_US
dc.identifier.issn0014-4819en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/144952-
dc.description.abstractThe present study addresses the role of vision for perception in determining the location of a target in far-aiming. Participants (N = 12) slid a disk toward a distant target embedded in illusory Judd figures. Additionally, in a perception task, participants indicated when a moving pointer reached the midpoint of the Judd figures. The number of hits, the number of misses to the left and to the right of the target, the sliding error (in mm) and perceptual judgment error (in mm) served as dependent variables. Results showed an illusory bias in sliding, the magnitude of which was comparable to the bias in the perception of target location. The determination of target location in far-aiming is thus based on relative metrics. We argue that vision for perception sets the boundary constraints for action and that within these constraints vision for action autonomously controls movement execution, but alternative accounts are discussed as well.en_US
dc.languageengen_US
dc.publisherSpringer Berlin / Heidelbergen_US
dc.relation.ispartofExperimental Brain Researchen_US
dc.rightsThe Author(s)en_US
dc.rightsThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.en_US
dc.subjectDorsal system-
dc.subjectFar-aiming-
dc.subjectJudd-illusion-
dc.subjectVentral system-
dc.subject.meshIllusions-
dc.subject.meshMotor Activity-
dc.subject.meshPsychomotor Performance-
dc.subject.meshTask Performance and Analysis-
dc.subject.meshVisual Perception-
dc.titleA Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perceptionen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.identifier.openurlhttp://library.hku.hk:4551/resserv?sid=springerlink&genre=article&atitle=A Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception&title=Experimental Brain Research&issn=00144819&date=2009-08-01&volume=197&issue=2& spage=199&authors=John van der Kamp, Hemke van Doorn, Rich S. W. Mastersen_US
dc.identifier.emailvan der Kamp, J: jvdkamp@hkucc.hku.hk-
dc.identifier.emailMasters, RSW: mastersr@hku.hk-
dc.identifier.authorityRich S. W. Masters=rp00935en_US
dc.description.naturepublished_or_final_versionen_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1007/s00221-009-1889-zen_US
dc.identifier.pmid19526357-
dc.identifier.pmcidPMC2713035-
dc.identifier.scopuseid_2-s2.0-68349130503en_US
dc.identifier.hkuros163627-
dc.relation.referencesAgliotti S, DeSouza JF, Goodale MA (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Cur Biol 5:679–685en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3en_US
dc.relation.referencesBruno N, Bernardis P, Gentilucci M (2008) Visually guided pointing, the Müller-Lyer illusion, and the functional interpretation of the dorsal-ventral split: conclusions from 33 independent studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:423–437en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.006en_US
dc.relation.referencesCrajé C, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B (2008) The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task. Exp Brain Res 185:53–62en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1130-xen_US
dc.relation.referencesEllis RR, Flanagan JR, Lederman SJ (1999) The influence of visual illusions on grasp position. Exp Brain Res 125:109–114en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1007/s002210050665en_US
dc.relation.referencesFleming J, Behrmann M (1998) Visuospatial neglect in normal subjects: altered spatial representations induced by a perceptual illusion. Neuropsychologia 36:469–475en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00131-0en_US
dc.relation.referencesGanel T, Tanzer M, Goodale MA (2008) A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: opposite effects of real and illusory size. Psychol Sci 19:221–225en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02071.xen_US
dc.relation.referencesGentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E et al (1996) Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 34:369–376en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00128-Xen_US
dc.relation.referencesGlover S (2004) Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behav Brain Sci 27:3–24en_US
dc.relation.referencesGlover S, Dixon P (2004) A step and a hop on the Müller-Lyer illusion: illusion effects on lower-limb movements. Exp Brain Res 154:504–512en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1687-yen_US
dc.relation.referencesGonzalez CLR, Ganel T, Whitwell RL et al (2008) Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychologia 46:624–631en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.006en_US
dc.relation.referencesGoodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8en_US
dc.relation.referencesLongo MR, Lourenco SF (2006) On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia 44:977–981en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003en_US
dc.relation.referencesMcCourt ME, Garlinghouse M (2000) Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex 36:715–731en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70548-3en_US
dc.relation.referencesMilner AD, Goodale MA (2008) Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46:774–785en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005en_US
dc.relation.referencesPost RB, Welch RB (1996) Is there dissociation of perceptual and motor responses to figural illusions? Percept 25:569–581en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1068/p250569en_US
dc.relation.referencesSmeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cujpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7en_US
dc.relation.referencesvan der Kamp J, Masters RSW (2008) The human Müller-Lyer illusion in goalkeeping. Percept 37:951–954en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1068/p6010en_US
dc.relation.referencesvan der Kamp J, Oudejans RDD, Savelsbergh GJP (2003) The development and learning of the visual control of movement: an ecological perspective. Inf Behav Dev 26:495–515en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002en_US
dc.relation.referencesvan Doorn H, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2007) Grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion: the contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 45:1939–1947en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008en_US
dc.relation.referencesVickers JN (1992) Gaze control in putting. Percept 21:117–132en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1068/p210117en_US
dc.relation.referencesVishton PM, Pea JG, Cutting JE, Nunez LN (1999) Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25:1659–1672en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1659en_US
dc.relation.referencesWestwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spat Vis 16:243–254en_US
dc.relation.referencesdoi: 10.1163/156856803322467518en_US
dc.relation.referencesvan der Kamp J, Rivas F, van Doorn H et al (2008) Ventral and dorsal contributions in visual anticipation in fast ball sports. Int J Sport Psychol 39:100–130en_US
dc.identifier.volume197en_US
dc.identifier.issue2en_US
dc.identifier.spage199en_US
dc.identifier.epage204en_US
dc.identifier.eissn1432-1106en_US
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000268744200010-
dc.description.otherSpringer Open Choice, 21 Feb 2012en_US
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridVan Der Kamp, J=7003734906-
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridVan Doorn, H=6701433317-
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridMasters, RSW=7102880488-
dc.identifier.citeulike5006845-
dc.identifier.issnl0014-4819-

Export via OAI-PMH Interface in XML Formats


OR


Export to Other Non-XML Formats