File Download
Links for fulltext
(May Require Subscription)
- Publisher Website: 10.1007/s00221-012-3007-x
- Scopus: eid_2-s2.0-84859162854
- PMID: 22278110
- WOS: WOS:000302247800010
- Find via
Supplementary
- Citations:
- Appears in Collections:
Article: Left, right, left, right, eyes to the front! Muller-Lyer bias in grasping is not a function of hand used, hand preferred or visual hemifield, but foveation does matter
Title | Left, right, left, right, eyes to the front! Muller-Lyer bias in grasping is not a function of hand used, hand preferred or visual hemifield, but foveation does matter |
---|---|
Authors | |
Keywords | Handedness Perception-action Peripheral visual field Visual hemifield Visual illusions |
Issue Date | 2012 |
Publisher | Springer Verlag. The Journal's web site is located at http://link.springer.de/link/service/journals/00221/index.htm |
Citation | Experimental Brain Research, 2012, v. 218 n. 1, p. 91-98 How to Cite? |
Abstract | We investigated whether the control of movement of the left hand is more likely to involve the use of allocentric information than movements performed with the right hand. Previous studies (Gonzalez et al. in J Neurophys 95:3496-3501, 2006; De Grave et al. in Exp Br Res 193:421-427, 2009) have reported contradictory findings in this respect. In the present study, right-handed participants (N = 12) and left-handed participants (N = 12) made right- and left-handed grasps to foveated objects and peripheral, non-foveated objects that were located in the right or left visual hemifield and embedded within a Müller-Lyer illusion. They were also asked to judge the size of the object by matching their hand aperture to its length. Hand apertures did not show significant differences in illusory bias as a function of hand used, handedness or visual hemifield. However, the illusory effect was significantly larger for perception than for action, and for the non-foveated compared to foveated objects. No significant illusory biases were found for reach movement times. These findings are consistent with the two-visual system model that holds that the use of allocentric information is more prominent in perception than in movement control. We propose that the increased involvement of allocentric information in movements toward peripheral, non-foveated objects may be a consequence of more awkward, less automatized grasps of nonfoveated than foveated objects. The current study does not support the conjecture that the control of left-handed and right-handed grasps is predicated on different sources of information. © 2012 The Author(s). |
Persistent Identifier | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/147119 |
ISSN | 2023 Impact Factor: 1.7 2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.613 |
ISI Accession Number ID | |
References |
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Van Der Kamp, J | en_HK |
dc.contributor.author | De Wit, MM | en_HK |
dc.contributor.author | Masters, RSW | en_HK |
dc.date.accessioned | 2012-05-28T08:18:41Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2012-05-28T08:18:41Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 2012 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.citation | Experimental Brain Research, 2012, v. 218 n. 1, p. 91-98 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.issn | 0014-4819 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/147119 | - |
dc.description.abstract | We investigated whether the control of movement of the left hand is more likely to involve the use of allocentric information than movements performed with the right hand. Previous studies (Gonzalez et al. in J Neurophys 95:3496-3501, 2006; De Grave et al. in Exp Br Res 193:421-427, 2009) have reported contradictory findings in this respect. In the present study, right-handed participants (N = 12) and left-handed participants (N = 12) made right- and left-handed grasps to foveated objects and peripheral, non-foveated objects that were located in the right or left visual hemifield and embedded within a Müller-Lyer illusion. They were also asked to judge the size of the object by matching their hand aperture to its length. Hand apertures did not show significant differences in illusory bias as a function of hand used, handedness or visual hemifield. However, the illusory effect was significantly larger for perception than for action, and for the non-foveated compared to foveated objects. No significant illusory biases were found for reach movement times. These findings are consistent with the two-visual system model that holds that the use of allocentric information is more prominent in perception than in movement control. We propose that the increased involvement of allocentric information in movements toward peripheral, non-foveated objects may be a consequence of more awkward, less automatized grasps of nonfoveated than foveated objects. The current study does not support the conjecture that the control of left-handed and right-handed grasps is predicated on different sources of information. © 2012 The Author(s). | en_HK |
dc.language | eng | en_US |
dc.publisher | Springer Verlag. The Journal's web site is located at http://link.springer.de/link/service/journals/00221/index.htm | en_HK |
dc.relation.ispartof | Experimental Brain Research | en_HK |
dc.rights | The Author(s) | en_US |
dc.rights | This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. | en_US |
dc.subject | Handedness | en_HK |
dc.subject | Perception-action | en_HK |
dc.subject | Peripheral visual field | en_HK |
dc.subject | Visual hemifield | en_HK |
dc.subject | Visual illusions | en_HK |
dc.title | Left, right, left, right, eyes to the front! Muller-Lyer bias in grasping is not a function of hand used, hand preferred or visual hemifield, but foveation does matter | en_HK |
dc.type | Article | en_HK |
dc.identifier.openurl | http://www.springerlink.com/link-out/?id=2104&code=U9T70810306192Q5&MUD=MP | en_US |
dc.identifier.email | Masters, RSW: mastersr@hku.hk | en_HK |
dc.identifier.authority | Masters, RSW=rp00935 | en_HK |
dc.description.nature | published_or_final_version | en_US |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1007/s00221-012-3007-x | en_HK |
dc.identifier.pmid | 22278110 | - |
dc.identifier.scopus | eid_2-s2.0-84859162854 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.hkuros | 207962 | - |
dc.identifier.hkuros | 210985 | - |
dc.relation.references | http://www.scopus.com/mlt/select.url?eid=2-s2.0-84859162854&selection=ref&src=s&origin=recordpage | en_HK |
dc.identifier.volume | 218 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.issue | 1 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.spage | 91 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.epage | 98 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.eissn | 1432-1106 | en_US |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:000302247800010 | - |
dc.publisher.place | Germany | en_HK |
dc.description.other | Springer Open Choice, 28 May 2012 | en_US |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Van Der Kamp, J=7003734906 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | De Wit, MM=35236753500 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Masters, RSW=7102880488 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.citeulike | 10310720 | - |
dc.identifier.issnl | 0014-4819 | - |