File Download
Links for fulltext
(May Require Subscription)
- Publisher Website: 10.1007/s00221-009-1889-z
- Scopus: eid_2-s2.0-68349130503
- PMID: 19526357
- WOS: WOS:000268744200010
- Find via
Supplementary
-
Bookmarks:
- CiteULike: 2
- Citations:
- Appears in Collections:
Article: A Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception
Title | A Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception |
---|---|
Authors | |
Keywords | Dorsal system Far-aiming Judd-illusion Ventral system |
Issue Date | 2009 |
Publisher | Springer Berlin / Heidelberg |
Citation | Experimental Brain Research, 2009, v. 197, n. 2, p. 199-204 How to Cite? |
Abstract | The present study addresses the role of vision for perception in determining the location of a target in far-aiming. Participants (N = 12) slid a disk toward a distant target embedded in illusory Judd figures. Additionally, in a perception task, participants indicated when a moving pointer reached the midpoint of the Judd figures. The number of hits, the number of misses to the left and to the right of the target, the sliding error (in mm) and perceptual judgment error (in mm) served as dependent variables. Results showed an illusory bias in sliding, the magnitude of which was comparable to the bias in the perception of target location. The determination of target location in far-aiming is thus based on relative metrics. We argue that vision for perception sets the boundary constraints for action and that within these constraints vision for action autonomously controls movement execution, but alternative accounts are discussed as well. |
Persistent Identifier | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/144952 |
ISSN | 2023 Impact Factor: 1.7 2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.613 |
PubMed Central ID | |
ISI Accession Number ID | |
References | Agliotti S, DeSouza JF, Goodale MA (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Cur Biol 5:679–685 doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3 Bruno N, Bernardis P, Gentilucci M (2008) Visually guided pointing, the Müller-Lyer illusion, and the functional interpretation of the dorsal-ventral split: conclusions from 33 independent studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:423–437 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.006 Crajé C, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B (2008) The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task. Exp Brain Res 185:53–62 doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1130-x Ellis RR, Flanagan JR, Lederman SJ (1999) The influence of visual illusions on grasp position. Exp Brain Res 125:109–114 doi: 10.1007/s002210050665 Fleming J, Behrmann M (1998) Visuospatial neglect in normal subjects: altered spatial representations induced by a perceptual illusion. Neuropsychologia 36:469–475 doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00131-0 Ganel T, Tanzer M, Goodale MA (2008) A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: opposite effects of real and illusory size. Psychol Sci 19:221–225 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02071.x Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E et al (1996) Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 34:369–376 doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00128-X Glover S, Dixon P (2004) A step and a hop on the Müller-Lyer illusion: illusion effects on lower-limb movements. Exp Brain Res 154:504–512 doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1687-y Gonzalez CLR, Ganel T, Whitwell RL et al (2008) Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychologia 46:624–631 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.006 Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25 doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8 Longo MR, Lourenco SF (2006) On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia 44:977–981 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003 McCourt ME, Garlinghouse M (2000) Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex 36:715–731 doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70548-3 Milner AD, Goodale MA (2008) Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46:774–785 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005 Post RB, Welch RB (1996) Is there dissociation of perceptual and motor responses to figural illusions? Percept 25:569–581 doi: 10.1068/p250569 Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cujpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7 van der Kamp J, Masters RSW (2008) The human Müller-Lyer illusion in goalkeeping. Percept 37:951–954 doi: 10.1068/p6010 van der Kamp J, Oudejans RDD, Savelsbergh GJP (2003) The development and learning of the visual control of movement: an ecological perspective. Inf Behav Dev 26:495–515 doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002 van Doorn H, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2007) Grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion: the contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 45:1939–1947 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008 Vickers JN (1992) Gaze control in putting. Percept 21:117–132 doi: 10.1068/p210117 Vishton PM, Pea JG, Cutting JE, Nunez LN (1999) Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25:1659–1672 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1659 Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spat Vis 16:243–254 doi: 10.1163/156856803322467518 |
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | van der Kamp, J | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | van Doorn, H | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Masters, RSW | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2012-02-21T05:44:56Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2012-02-21T05:44:56Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 2009 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Experimental Brain Research, 2009, v. 197, n. 2, p. 199-204 | en_US |
dc.identifier.issn | 0014-4819 | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/144952 | - |
dc.description.abstract | The present study addresses the role of vision for perception in determining the location of a target in far-aiming. Participants (N = 12) slid a disk toward a distant target embedded in illusory Judd figures. Additionally, in a perception task, participants indicated when a moving pointer reached the midpoint of the Judd figures. The number of hits, the number of misses to the left and to the right of the target, the sliding error (in mm) and perceptual judgment error (in mm) served as dependent variables. Results showed an illusory bias in sliding, the magnitude of which was comparable to the bias in the perception of target location. The determination of target location in far-aiming is thus based on relative metrics. We argue that vision for perception sets the boundary constraints for action and that within these constraints vision for action autonomously controls movement execution, but alternative accounts are discussed as well. | en_US |
dc.language | eng | en_US |
dc.publisher | Springer Berlin / Heidelberg | en_US |
dc.relation.ispartof | Experimental Brain Research | en_US |
dc.rights | The Author(s) | en_US |
dc.rights | This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. | en_US |
dc.subject | Dorsal system | - |
dc.subject | Far-aiming | - |
dc.subject | Judd-illusion | - |
dc.subject | Ventral system | - |
dc.subject.mesh | Illusions | - |
dc.subject.mesh | Motor Activity | - |
dc.subject.mesh | Psychomotor Performance | - |
dc.subject.mesh | Task Performance and Analysis | - |
dc.subject.mesh | Visual Perception | - |
dc.title | A Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.identifier.openurl | http://library.hku.hk:4551/resserv?sid=springerlink&genre=article&atitle=A Judd illusion in far-aiming: evidence of a contribution to action by vision for perception&title=Experimental Brain Research&issn=00144819&date=2009-08-01&volume=197&issue=2& spage=199&authors=John van der Kamp, Hemke van Doorn, Rich S. W. Masters | en_US |
dc.identifier.email | van der Kamp, J: jvdkamp@hkucc.hku.hk | - |
dc.identifier.email | Masters, RSW: mastersr@hku.hk | - |
dc.identifier.authority | Rich S. W. Masters=rp00935 | en_US |
dc.description.nature | published_or_final_version | en_US |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1007/s00221-009-1889-z | en_US |
dc.identifier.pmid | 19526357 | - |
dc.identifier.pmcid | PMC2713035 | - |
dc.identifier.scopus | eid_2-s2.0-68349130503 | en_US |
dc.identifier.hkuros | 163627 | - |
dc.relation.references | Agliotti S, DeSouza JF, Goodale MA (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Cur Biol 5:679–685 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Bruno N, Bernardis P, Gentilucci M (2008) Visually guided pointing, the Müller-Lyer illusion, and the functional interpretation of the dorsal-ventral split: conclusions from 33 independent studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:423–437 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.006 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Crajé C, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B (2008) The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task. Exp Brain Res 185:53–62 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1130-x | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Ellis RR, Flanagan JR, Lederman SJ (1999) The influence of visual illusions on grasp position. Exp Brain Res 125:109–114 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s002210050665 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Fleming J, Behrmann M (1998) Visuospatial neglect in normal subjects: altered spatial representations induced by a perceptual illusion. Neuropsychologia 36:469–475 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00131-0 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Ganel T, Tanzer M, Goodale MA (2008) A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: opposite effects of real and illusory size. Psychol Sci 19:221–225 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02071.x | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E et al (1996) Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 34:369–376 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00128-X | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S (2004) Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behav Brain Sci 27:3–24 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S, Dixon P (2004) A step and a hop on the Müller-Lyer illusion: illusion effects on lower-limb movements. Exp Brain Res 154:504–512 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1687-y | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Gonzalez CLR, Ganel T, Whitwell RL et al (2008) Practice makes perfect, but only with the right hand: sensitivity to perceptual illusions with awkward grasps decreases with practice in the right but not the left hand. Neuropsychologia 46:624–631 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.006 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Longo MR, Lourenco SF (2006) On the nature of near space: effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia 44:977–981 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | McCourt ME, Garlinghouse M (2000) Asymmetries of visuospatial attention are modulated by viewing distance and visual field elevation: pseudoneglect in peripersonal and extrapersonal space. Cortex 36:715–731 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70548-3 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Milner AD, Goodale MA (2008) Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46:774–785 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Post RB, Welch RB (1996) Is there dissociation of perceptual and motor responses to figural illusions? Percept 25:569–581 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1068/p250569 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cujpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | van der Kamp J, Masters RSW (2008) The human Müller-Lyer illusion in goalkeeping. Percept 37:951–954 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1068/p6010 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | van der Kamp J, Oudejans RDD, Savelsbergh GJP (2003) The development and learning of the visual control of movement: an ecological perspective. Inf Behav Dev 26:495–515 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.002 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | van Doorn H, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2007) Grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion: the contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 45:1939–1947 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Vickers JN (1992) Gaze control in putting. Percept 21:117–132 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1068/p210117 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Vishton PM, Pea JG, Cutting JE, Nunez LN (1999) Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25:1659–1672 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.6.1659 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spat Vis 16:243–254 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1163/156856803322467518 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | van der Kamp J, Rivas F, van Doorn H et al (2008) Ventral and dorsal contributions in visual anticipation in fast ball sports. Int J Sport Psychol 39:100–130 | en_US |
dc.identifier.volume | 197 | en_US |
dc.identifier.issue | 2 | en_US |
dc.identifier.spage | 199 | en_US |
dc.identifier.epage | 204 | en_US |
dc.identifier.eissn | 1432-1106 | en_US |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:000268744200010 | - |
dc.description.other | Springer Open Choice, 21 Feb 2012 | en_US |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Van Der Kamp, J=7003734906 | - |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Van Doorn, H=6701433317 | - |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Masters, RSW=7102880488 | - |
dc.identifier.citeulike | 5006845 | - |
dc.identifier.issnl | 0014-4819 | - |