File Download
There are no files associated with this item.
Links for fulltext
(May Require Subscription)
- Publisher Website: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02876.x
- Scopus: eid_2-s2.0-0025501895
- PMID: 2245739
- WOS: WOS:A1990ED70000015
- Find via
Supplementary
- Citations:
- Appears in Collections:
Article: The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: to honor or not to honor?
Title | The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: to honor or not to honor? |
---|---|
Authors | |
Issue Date | 1990 |
Publisher | Blackwell Publishing, Inc. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/journals/CDEV |
Citation | Child Development, 1990, v. 61 n. 5, p. 1474-1490 How to Cite? |
Abstract | According to Markman and Wachtel, children assume that nouns pick out mutually exclusive object categories, and so each object should have only one category label. While this assumption can be useful in word learning, it is not entirely reliable. Therefore, children need to learn when to and when not to make this assumption. 6 studies examined whether knowledge about hierarchical organization of categories and about cross-language equivalents for object labels can help children limit their use of this assumption appropriately. These studies revealed that adults as well as children resisted assigning 2 novel names to the same object in some situations. By age 4, children also seemed to know enough about categorization to accept 2 names for an object if the names picked out categories from different levels of a hierarchy (e.g., animal and lemur) but not if they picked out categories from the same level (e.g., lemur and seal). Moreover, monolingual as well as bilingual children seemed to know enough about languages to accept 2 names for the same object if the names clearly came from different languages. Together, these findings suggest that even preschool children can make use of knowledge about language and categorization to fine tune the mutual exclusivity assumption in order to use it effectively in word learning. |
Persistent Identifier | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/132044 |
ISSN | 2023 Impact Factor: 3.9 2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 2.082 |
ISI Accession Number ID |
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Au, TK | en_HK |
dc.contributor.author | Glusman, M | en_HK |
dc.date.accessioned | 2011-03-07T08:27:48Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2011-03-07T08:27:48Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 1990 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.citation | Child Development, 1990, v. 61 n. 5, p. 1474-1490 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.issn | 0009-3920 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/132044 | - |
dc.description.abstract | According to Markman and Wachtel, children assume that nouns pick out mutually exclusive object categories, and so each object should have only one category label. While this assumption can be useful in word learning, it is not entirely reliable. Therefore, children need to learn when to and when not to make this assumption. 6 studies examined whether knowledge about hierarchical organization of categories and about cross-language equivalents for object labels can help children limit their use of this assumption appropriately. These studies revealed that adults as well as children resisted assigning 2 novel names to the same object in some situations. By age 4, children also seemed to know enough about categorization to accept 2 names for an object if the names picked out categories from different levels of a hierarchy (e.g., animal and lemur) but not if they picked out categories from the same level (e.g., lemur and seal). Moreover, monolingual as well as bilingual children seemed to know enough about languages to accept 2 names for the same object if the names clearly came from different languages. Together, these findings suggest that even preschool children can make use of knowledge about language and categorization to fine tune the mutual exclusivity assumption in order to use it effectively in word learning. | en_HK |
dc.language | eng | - |
dc.publisher | Blackwell Publishing, Inc. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/journals/CDEV | en_HK |
dc.relation.ispartof | Child development | en_HK |
dc.rights | The definitive version is available at www.blackwell-synergy.com | - |
dc.title | The principle of mutual exclusivity in word learning: to honor or not to honor? | en_HK |
dc.type | Article | en_HK |
dc.identifier.openurl | http://library.hku.hk:4550/resserv?sid=HKU:IR&issn=0009-3920&volume=61&issue=5&spage=1474&epage=1490&date=1990&atitle=THE+PRINCIPLE+OF+MUTUAL+EXCLUSIVITY+IN+WORD+LEARNING+-+TO+HONOR+OR+NOT+TO+HONOR | - |
dc.identifier.email | Au, TK:terryau@hkucc.hku.hk | en_HK |
dc.identifier.authority | Au, TK=rp00580 | en_HK |
dc.description.nature | link_to_subscribed_fulltext | - |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02876.x | - |
dc.identifier.pmid | 2245739 | - |
dc.identifier.scopus | eid_2-s2.0-0025501895 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.volume | 61 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.issue | 5 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.spage | 1474 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.epage | 1490 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:A1990ED70000015 | - |
dc.publisher.place | United States | en_HK |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Au, TK=9435174900 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.scopusauthorid | Glusman, M=7003639403 | en_HK |
dc.identifier.issnl | 0009-3920 | - |