File Download

There are no files associated with this item.

  Links for fulltext
     (May Require Subscription)
Supplementary

Article: What Word Learners Do When Input Contradicts the Mutual Exclusivity Assumption

TitleWhat Word Learners Do When Input Contradicts the Mutual Exclusivity Assumption
Authors
Issue Date1996
PublisherBlackwell Publishing, Inc. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/journals/CDEV
Citation
Child Development, 1996, v. 67 n. 6, p. 3120-3134 How to Cite?
AbstractIn 4 studies, 3-to 5-year-olds heard 2 novel English labels each applied to the same novel object by a different adult. In all 4 studies, about half of the children accepted both labels, suggesting that hearing 2 labels applied to an object offers strong enough evidence for some to override mutual exclusivity. Nonetheless, about half of the children honored mutual exclusivity and hence accepted only 1 label; they also seemed to keep both labels in mind for at least a few minutes and settled on whichever label they re-encountered first. This strategy allows children to make an informed choice between 2 apparently equally good labels, without straining their limited memory capacities.
Persistent Identifierhttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/132040
ISSN
2023 Impact Factor: 3.9
2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 2.082
ISI Accession Number ID
References

 

DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorSavage, SLen_HK
dc.contributor.authorAu, TKFen_HK
dc.date.accessioned2011-03-07T08:19:28Z-
dc.date.available2011-03-07T08:19:28Z-
dc.date.issued1996en_HK
dc.identifier.citationChild Development, 1996, v. 67 n. 6, p. 3120-3134en_HK
dc.identifier.issn0009-3920en_HK
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/132040-
dc.description.abstractIn 4 studies, 3-to 5-year-olds heard 2 novel English labels each applied to the same novel object by a different adult. In all 4 studies, about half of the children accepted both labels, suggesting that hearing 2 labels applied to an object offers strong enough evidence for some to override mutual exclusivity. Nonetheless, about half of the children honored mutual exclusivity and hence accepted only 1 label; they also seemed to keep both labels in mind for at least a few minutes and settled on whichever label they re-encountered first. This strategy allows children to make an informed choice between 2 apparently equally good labels, without straining their limited memory capacities.en_HK
dc.languageeng-
dc.publisherBlackwell Publishing, Inc. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/journals/CDEVen_HK
dc.relation.ispartofChild Developmenten_HK
dc.rightsThe definitive version is available at www.blackwell-synergy.com-
dc.titleWhat Word Learners Do When Input Contradicts the Mutual Exclusivity Assumptionen_HK
dc.typeArticleen_HK
dc.identifier.openurlhttp://library.hku.hk:4550/resserv?sid=HKU:IR&issn=0009-3920&volume=67&issue=6&spage=3120&epage=3134&date=1996&atitle=What+word+learners+do+when+input+contradicts+the+mutual+exclusivity+assumption-
dc.identifier.emailAu, TKF:terryau@hkucc.hku.hken_HK
dc.identifier.authorityAu, TKF=rp00580en_HK
dc.description.naturelink_to_subscribed_fulltext-
dc.identifier.doi10.2307/1131770-
dc.identifier.pmid9071773-
dc.identifier.scopuseid_2-s2.0-0030334624en_HK
dc.relation.referenceshttp://www.scopus.com/mlt/select.url?eid=2-s2.0-0030334624&selection=ref&src=s&origin=recordpageen_HK
dc.identifier.volume67en_HK
dc.identifier.issue6en_HK
dc.identifier.spage3120en_HK
dc.identifier.epage3134en_HK
dc.identifier.isiWOS:A1996WN23500029-
dc.publisher.placeUnited Statesen_HK
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridSavage, SL=7103218437en_HK
dc.identifier.scopusauthoridAu, TKF=9435174900en_HK
dc.identifier.issnl0009-3920-

Export via OAI-PMH Interface in XML Formats


OR


Export to Other Non-XML Formats